Advice from the
field
Passing
on a Sense of Place and Traditional
Ecological Knowledge between Generations:
a Primer for Native American Museum
Educators and Community-Based Cultural
Education Projects
Compiled and edited by Gary Paul Nabhan
Introduction
Many communities, especially those on
Indian reservations near rapidly-growing
urban areas, have demonstrated their
commitment to ensuring that their sense
of place, as well as their traditional
ecological knowledge and values about
native plants and animals, continue to be
passed on to their children. The passing
on of knowledge by word of mouth, and the
passing on of values and skills
associated with traditional cultures,
while still continuing, has often been
disrupted by a number of external
pressures. Childrens attitudes,
knowledge and behaviors toward animals,
plants and their habitats may therefore
be very different from those of the
generations preceding them (Kellert and
Westervelt 1977).
Instead of being regularly involved in
learning from community elders about
their peoples traditional ways of
living well in the desert, most children
today spend more time in schools, in
transit to and from schools, and in front
of the television (Nabhan and St. Antoine
1993). Over the last fifty years, more
parents have abandoned traditional
subsistence activities to work away from
home for wages, and therefore have less
time to teach their children their native
language, customs, and skills (Zitnow
1990). It also appears that Native
American children in the Southwest now
spend less time alone or with peers in
the desert, directly involved with the
plants and animals native to their
homeland (Nabhan 1997; Rosenberg 1997).
In a recent survey of 27 Tohono
Oodham schoolchildren in southern
Arizona, only 37% of the eight to
fourteen year olds claimed that they had
ever gathered wild foods, and 44% said
they had been hunting with their parents
or grandparents. However, 78% said that
they had watched TV shows about wildlife,
56% had read books about wildlife within
the previous year, and 74% have been to
zoos, museums or botanical gardens in the
last two years (Nabhan and Tanner,
unpublished data). These and other
indicators suggest that children
even those living in the most remote
rural areas are now influenced by
many sources of information about the
natural world over and above what their
parents and grandparents teach them, or
what they learn by direct hands-on
experience. Figures 1 and 2 present data
from a cross-cultural comparison of
Anglo-, Mexican-, Oodham- and
Yaqui-American students living in rural
areas of southern Arizona and adjacent
Sonora, all within miles of National
Parks. These preliminary data clearly
indicate that todays schoolchildren
do not necessarily experience the range
of activities upon which their
cultures traditional sense of place
was formerly based.
It has been argued that traditional
farming, hunting and gathering skills are
no longer needed for survival as they
once were. Yet some communities have
recognized that such family-based
activities serve as the most important
vehicles for teaching native language,
traditional stories and community values
that reinforce a distinctive sense of
place (Ohmagari and Berkes 1997). Skills
such as traditional food gathering and
processing, or basket making and hide
tanning, can only be learned by
hands-on instruction from
elders, and there are specialized
vocabularies which go with each of these
activities. It appears that the native
terms associated with place-based
subsistence activities such as tanning
hides or crafting wood implements are the
realm within native languages which are
most rapidly being lost (Zepeda 1983).
Put in the context that 90% of the
worlds native languages will be
lost within the next century (Krauss
1992), the outlook for linguistically
encoded traditional knowledge about place
is not good.
There are numerous programs around
North America that are encouraging native
language retention, but few of them
reinforce native language use in
conjunction with traditional knowledge
about the local environment through
community visits to sacred places, plant
gathering grounds, hunting camps or
springs. The Sense of Place Project of
the Arizona-Sonora Desert Museum has
recently sponsored such outings in
collaboration with Seri, Oodham,
and Hispanic communities, in some cases
taking children to such places for the
very first time in their lives. The Sense
of Place Project is committed to
recognizing, celebrating and reinforcing
local knowledge of place, plants and
animals in the Sonoran Desert
borderlands. In particular, one of our
objectives is to work with communities
wishing to pass on traditional ecological
knowledge, and to reinforce it through
their own communities institutions
such as tribal museums and cultural
preservation programs.
However, there are few communities
which know the extent to which
traditional knowledge about place, plants
and animals is still being passed on from
generation to generation or, conversely,
the magnitude of loss of language,
knowledge and hands-on experience which
has already occurred (Ohmagari and Berkes
1997; Zent in press). There is currently
no standard methodology for determining
the rate or extent of traditional
knowledge retention or loss about place,
nor any surefire techniques for
preventing or mitigating such losses
which work in all communities (Ohmagari
and Berkes 1997). Nevertheless, by
carefully choosing one or more of the
intergenerational survey methods
suggested below, and establishing a
benchmark assessment, communities can
better determine the challenges they face
in ensuring that effective
culturally-sensitive environmental
education occurs.
Determining goals, choosing
appropriate methods
There are several possible objectives
that a community may have for sponsoring
an intergenerational survey of
traditional knowledge retention or loss.
Each objective may require a different
kind of survey, or a combination of
several different kinds of interviews and
assessments. We will list some of the
many possibilities below, providing short
synopses of appropriate methods to go
with them.
Retention of native
terms/names for plants, animals, and
places
As part of comprehensive
community-based surveys of native
language loss or retention, language
educators may wish to see if there is
differential loss of terms relating to
the local environment, to its biota, and
to traditional subsistence skills (Zepeda
1983; Zepeda and Hill 1995). For
Oodham speakers, Hill and Zepeda
presented a scrapbook of over a hundred
photos from magazines and books which
illustrated local plant and animal life
and subsistence activities, then asked
native speakers in Oodham to name
or describe the objects and organisms
while being tape-recorded. More than
eighty plants and animals were pictured.
They compared the names given by
different ages of women to come up with a
measure of lexemic (name) loss, but also
analyzed the extent to which dialect
distinctions were maintained, or loan
words used.
One flaw with this method was that
some of the photos were not detailed
enough for interviewees to distinguish
and name different species of prickly
pear, or different kinds of beans. In
addition, some sets of photos were used
to elicit names of life-forms (such as
fish) as opposed to showing local species
(such as desert pupfish.)
Similarly, in a pilot survey of
Oodham children paired with their
grandparents in the Ajo, Arizona area,
Nabhan and St. Antoine (1993) used a
booklet of 20 line drawings of common
plants and animals. However, it was
difficult for all those interviewed to
determine which black bird
was illustrated a boat-tailed
grackle, a common raven, or a
Brewers blackbird. If photos or
drawings are to be used in such surveys,
they should be of plants or animals not
closely related to other local species.
For instance, showing drawings of jojoba
and saguaro might prove more productive
than showing two kinds of mesquite trees,
since the traits by which Oodham
elders distinguish the two mesquites
cannot often be seen in photos. For
Rosenbergs (1997) interviews of
Seri children, Arizona-Sonora Desert
Museum photo archives were searched to
obtain 40 color photos which showed
distinctive local (often endemic) species
close-up, with most distinguishing
features visible. Even so, the identities
of certain species of lizards remained so
difficult to determine that the Seri had
no consensus about their names (Rosenberg
1997), perhaps because they are
traditionally identified as much by
behavior and habitat as by morphology.
In order to overcome problems posed by
using photos and drawings, Zent (in
press) took Piaroa schoolchildren on
walks through a five yard wide and 150
yard long transect of old growth forest
located thirty minutes (by foot) from
their town. There, he marked 50 different
kinds of plants, and asked those
interviewed to name and discuss the
cultural significance of each marked
plant along the transect. His forty-four
respondents sorted into two groups, with
a much higher competence level displayed
among those over thirty years of age who
grew up in such forests. Thus, the older
the respondent, the more competent he was
in knowing Piaroan names for plants;
however, the more one participated in
formal (Spanish-language) education, the
less likely it was that the person would
have the same level of naming competency
that an unschooled person of the same age
retained. In general, Zent felt that
showing respondents live plants growing
in the local environment meant that
methodological problems in obtaining
near-perfect identifications for all
species were virtually eliminated.
Nevertheless, such surveys show that
even Oodham women in their twenties
and thirties recall only a fraction of
the native names for plants and animals
which older women in their communities
still recall (Zepeda and Hill 1995). In
the pilot survey near Ajo, Oodham
children knew only a third of the native
names which their grandparents knew for
the most common plants and animals in
their local environments (Nabhan and St.
Antoine 1993). These initial surveys had
small sample sizes and should be done in
more communities before any final
conclusions are made regarding
ethnobiological knowledge retention among
the Oodham. Nevertheless, they
suggest that losses are occurring even in
a group such as the Oodham which
continues to have thousands of native
language speakers. Fortunately, programs
such as Tohono Oodham Community
Action are giving young people
opportunities to reacquaint themselves
with native names for basketry and food
plants, and for processing skills.
Retention of traditional
knowledge and values of local wildlife
Educators may wish to know whether the
way in which science is taught in schools
is changing childrens values and
perceptions of local animals. For
example, a science teacher may lecture
students about how horned lizards are
harmless animals, but the childrens
grandparents may simultaneously teach
them that these creatures are dangerous
and may cause human illness if
mistreated.
Rosenberg (1997) devised a means of
eliciting information from children and
elders in the Seri community with regard
to such knowledge, beliefs and values.
She asked interviewees to sort photos of
animals into groupings of which animals
were good, beautiful or liked, versus
those which are considered bad, ugly,
dangerous or hated. She also asked the
Seri about which animals were considered
edible or poisonous. Finally, with regard
to ceremonially important animals such as
the leatherback turtle, she asked if
children had been to the ceremony,
whether they believed that leatherbacks
can talk with the Seri, and whether
leatherbacks can or should be eaten. Only
two children under twenty had
participated in the ceremony; among the
20 to 39 year olds, only 10% think that
leatherback meat is eaten at ceremonies;
this percentage increased to 44% in the
next age group (40-59), and to 50% among
those over 60 years of age (Rosenberg
1997).
Where there are differences of opinion
especially between different age
groups the usual assumption is
that the eldest groups answer is
the correct one. However,
there may even be differences of opinion
among elders, as there is in this
fifty/fifty split among Seri elders.
Rosenberg (1997) did not attempt to use
consensus analysis (Romney, Weller and
Batchelder 1986) to determine which
answer is shared by most members of a
culture, but this additional analytical
tool has proven useful in other studies
(Zent in press). However, it may be valid
to give more weight to consensus among
elders relative to consensus among
schoolchildren for certain kinds of
information.
Retention of traditional
ecological knowledge about natural
resources
Traditional hunters, gatherers,
herbalists, and farmers know far more
than the mere names of plants and
animals; they also know a lot about the
ecological relationships among plants and
animals, and how certain ecological
processes such as fire or flood affect
their distributions. Although all these
relationships and processes are not
necessarily given names, they are known
in a way which can be elicited by
interviews. Simple questions such as
Where does this animal live?
or What animal eats this
plant? can help document such
traditional ecological knowledge. In
addition, Rosenberg (1997) asked children
to compare the hunting and nesting habits
of selected species (ospreys and
pelicans) which are locally abundant.
Because a single species of animal may
eat many things or nest at different
places within its territory, the range of
answers may be different for children who
stay close to a single home, versus
adults who had a more mobile lifestyle
while growing up. Any differences in
outcomes from different age groups should
be carefully interpreted.
Retention of traditional
subsistence skills
Because the major means by which all
traditional ecological knowledge is
transmitted is through hands-on
involvement in food-getting,
basket-making, and land management,
Ohmagari and Berkes (1997) have focused
on how, when and to what extent children
are learning the bush skills
associated with their communitys
heritage. They cite a learning sequence
that they believe most children go
through in learning the land- or
water-based traditions of their
community. The child becomes familiar
with the fact that elders participate in
an activity such as hunting waterfowl or
harvesting willow for basketry; they then
observe the hunting or harvesting. Next,
they take initial steps in participation,
assisting with the most basic tasks. They
are then explicitly shown the entire
sequence of a process by an elder, and
are next asked to perform the sequence
under the supervision of an adult.
Finally, they begin to experiment with
innovations so as to personalize the
task, and they become equal partners with
their instructors.
In two rural Cree
communities, Ohmagari and Berkes (1997)
asked women three questions about each of
93 particular bush skills
involved in foraging, fishing, hunting
and crafts-making:
Did you learn the
particular skill?
If yes, who was your major teacher?
How old were you when you learned the
skill?
Historically, most Cree
girls learned such skills from their
parents, grandparents and aunts. Today,
it is becoming more common for women to
learn these skills only after they have
married, and then it is from those of
their in-laws who remain most active in
subsistence activities. It may be that a
revival of interest in traditional
ecological skills and knowledge is now
going on among Cree individuals who
missed exposure to such activities while
they were young. Ironically, boarding
school students were among those most
active in learning these skills when they
came home on vacation for extended
periods, while the children who stayed at
home and went to local schools tended to
take the skills for granted.
For better or worse, many
subsistence skills tend to be taught
within a single gender, from mother or
mother-in-law to daughter or
daughter-in-law. There are exceptions of
course; both Oodham boys and girls
have learned certain kinds of
basketry-making techniques. Nevertheless,
it may be relevant in some communities to
discuss with boys and girls different
lists of traditional skills, with some
items on both lists.
The following list is
offered as examples of questions about
some traditional subsistence skills
commonly found throughout rural desert
communities:
When is the best time
of the year to harvest yucca leaves
for baskets?
How do you prepare gourds for making
rattles, ladles or masks?
What is the best wood to burn for
baking clay pottery?
Why shouldnt you hunt rabbits
during the summertime?
When is the best time of year to cut
mesquite for making termite-resistant
posts?
When can desert tortoises be found
sleeping in caves or crevices?
How can you remove spines or stickers
from prickly pear cactus so that you
can eat them?
How big should wild greens (quelites)
be when you pick them?
If you harvest creosote
(greasewood/hediondilla), do you take
the root or the green branches?
If you harvest night-blooming cactus,
do you take the root or the branches?
Retention of
traditional songs, stories and vernacular
maps that reinforce a sense of place
Traditional stories about
places and songs about plants and animals
continue to be shared in most
communities, whether or not they are
still offered in native dialects. In
certain cases, these cannot be shared
with outsiders, at least not during all
seasons. Nevertheless, it is possible to
simply ask children and adults if they
themselves have sung songs or told
stories about coyotes, tortoises, eagles,
cacti or other species.
When permissible, asking
for a description or summary of the song
or story helps to verify that children
are speaking of the same narrative that
elders are speaking of, and not one from
a Coyote and Roadrunner cartoon.
Rosenberg (1997) was able to determine
that Seri elders knew two and half times
as many songs about animals as did the
children, even though there was not
nearly as much difference in their
competency in simply naming the animals
in Seri. Only a third of the children and
teens could say a tongue twister about
coral snake coloration that was performed
by virtually all Seri over the age of
forty.
With regard to sense of
place, it is not merely the names of
particular locations that matter; it is
also the events that took place there and
the stories about them (Basso 1996).
Certain elements which modern mapmakers
would never include have great prominence
on hand-drawn vernacular maps
by traditional peoples (Hine and Hill
1986). For instance, eighteen of
twenty-four Seri mapmakers marked the
area where their afterbirth was buried,
and ten of them centered their maps of
their land (terreño) on the exact place
where their afterbirth was buried.
Prominent features on the horizon and
along the coast were more important than
nearby towns, geopolitical boundaries or
even roads (Hine and Hill 1986). It would
be interesting to interview Apache youth
regarding their knowledge of the stories
recorded by Basso (1996) for the hundreds
of place names in the Cibecue area.
However, recent place-name work with the
Hia-ced Oodham in southwestern
Arizona suggests that few individuals
under 50 years of age have ever had
access to the majority of traditional
sites in their former homeland, since
much of it has become a bombing range
where public access is restricted.
When asking community
members to draw maps of their homeland,
it is best not to give them a base map,
or to predetermine which kinds of
cultural or natural features should be
mapped. The more open-ended the
invitation is, the closer the drawn map
may be to the individuals own
cognitive map.
Evaluating
results, determining educational
interventions
Regardless of which
methods are used to determine the degree
of intergenerational retention of
traditional knowledge, it should be
explained to the communities involved
that these surveys are only first
approximations intended to stimulate more
reflection, discussion and educational
action. Ultimately, the communitys
collective feelings not the
statistics alone are the best way
to determine whether traditional sense of
place and relationship to other creatures
is being lost, retained or revived.
Interviews and surveys should be thought
of as means to catalyze more community
discussion of the importance of language
and land-use traditions, and what is at
risk if these traditions are lost.
Communities need not
despair if they become aware that changes
are occurring at a much more rapid rate
than they had previously assumed. Many
communities have recently begun to
experiment with means to slow or avert
the losses of traditions. Immersion
schools and after-school tutorials with
elders have proven effective in slowing
native language loss, or even reviving
language use. Summer camps and field
trips focussed on teaching children
native traditions have been successful
among a variety of cultures, from the
Gwichiin on the Arctic Circle in
Alaska (Gildart 1997), the Haisla on the
Pacific temperate rainforest coast of
British Columbia, and the Tohono
Oodham in southern Arizona deserts.
The Arizona-Sonora Desert Museum and the
Ethnobiology and Conservation Team
recently sponsored a series of field
trips to traditionally-significant
places, where Seri elders knowledgeable
about songs and stories of those places
shared them with children who had never
before had access to those places
(Rosenberg and Nabhan 1997).
Obviously, there are many
other strategies for preserving cultural
traditions about place other than the
ones briefly mentioned here. And yet, we
see many community organizations
such as TOCA on the Tohono Oodham
Reservation embracing the notion
that neither native language nor stories
can survive unless traditional
interactions with the natural world
persist to give them context, their sense
of place. As TOCAs Tristan Reader
(1997) has recently written,
... it is not
enough to preserve a language, its words
and its linguistic structures, its images
and grammar. In order for a language to
be truly alive and vital, we must also
preserve the subjects of discussion. A
living language means a language that is
lived, a language that grows out of the
ways in which people make their living
and their meanings ... Cultural
preservation requires that we rejuvenate
traditional food systems, local economies
and ways of interacting with the natural
world.
We encourage communities,
their schools and museums, to sponsor
means by which children learn not only
their native language, crafts and
ceremonies, but also the subsistence
activities in the natural world from
which these artistic expressions emerged.
We welcome hearing of
activities and programs which have been
successful in helping elders pass on
their traditional knowledge and sense of
place to younger generations.
Literature
cited
- Basso, K. 1996. Wisdom
Sits in Places. Albuquerque,
University of New Mexico Press
- .Edwards, J. 1995.
Multilingualism. New
York, Penguin Books.
- Gildart, R.C.
1997. Gwichin: we are the
people. Native Peoples
11(2): 76-82.
- Hine, C. and J.
Hill. 1986. Seri Concepts of
Place. Unpublished paper
presented at the Southwestern
Anthropological
Associations annual
meeting, Las Vegas, Nevada, March
28, 1986.
- Kellert, S. and M.
O. Westervelt. 1977. Childrens
Attitudes, Knowledge and
Behaviors Toward Animals.
Phase V. U.S. Fish and Wildlife
Service, Washington, D.C.
- Krauss, M. 1992.
The worlds languages in
crisis. Language 68(1):
4-10.
- Nabhan, G.P. 1997.
Children in touch, creatures in
story. Pages 59-80 in Cultures
of Habitat. Washington, DC,
Counterpoint Press.
- Nabhan, G.P. and
S. St. Antoine. 1993. The loss of
floral and faunal story, the
extinction of experience. Pp.
229-250 in S. R. Kellert and E.O.
Wilson, editors, The
Biophilia Hypothesis.
Washington, DC, Island Press.
- Nabhan, G.P. and
T. Tanner. n.d. Results from
Oodham high school student
biology survey, 1992, Sells,
Arizona. Unpublished data.
- Ohmagari, K. And
F. Berkes. 1997. Transmission of
indigenous knowledge and bush
skills among the Western James
Bay Cree women of subarctic
Canada. Human Ecology
25(2): 197-222.
- Plotkin, Mark.
1997. Personal communication,
Arlington, Ethnobiology and
Conservation Team.
- Reader, T. 1997. Recontextualizing
culture: some reflections on the
material roots of language and
culture. Paper presented to
the Lannan Foundation by Tohono
Oodham Community Action
(TOCA), Tucson, Arizona.
- Romney, A.K., S.C.
Weller, and W.H. Batchelder.
1986. Culture as consensus: a
theory of culture and informant
accuracy. American
Anthropologist 88(2):
313-338.
- Rosenberg, J.
1997. Documenting and
Revitalizing Traditional
Ecological Knowledge: The Seri
Curriculum Project. Tucson,
University of Arizona
Masters Thesis.
- Rosenberg, J. and
G.P. Nabhan. 1997. Where ancient
stories guide children home. Natural
History 7: 263-268.
- Zent, S. In press.
The quandry of conserving
ethnoecological knowledge: a
Piaroa example. in T. Gragson and
B. Blount, editors,
Ethnoecology: Knowledge,
Resources and Rights. Iowa
City, University of Iowa Press,
- Zepeda, O. 1983. A
Papago Grammar. Tucson,
University of Arizona Press.
- Zepeda, O. and J.
Hill. 1995. Papago Dialect
Survey. Unpublished, Tucson,
University of Arizona.
- Zitnow, J.D. 1990.
A comparison of time Ojibway
adolescents spend with
parents/elders in the 1930s and
1980s. American Indian and
Alaska Native Mental Health
Research 3(3): 7-16.
This
article is taken from a manuscript
submitted to PPH by Gary Paul Nabhan,
Director of Science Outreach at the
Arizona-Sonora Desert Museum.
Contact: Gary Paul Nabhan,
Arizona-Desert Museum, 2021 N. Kenney
Road, Tucson, Arizona 85743-8918,
USA; Tel. +1.520.8831380, Fax
+1.520.8832500 Website http://www.desert.net/museum/
BACK
Documenting and
Revitalizing Traditional
Ecological Knowledge: Seri Survey
by Janice Rosenberg
One of the missions of
the Transboundary Sense of Place
Projects has been to determine
whether local knowledge about the Sonoran
Desert region is continuing to be passed
down from generation to generation. One
tool used toward this goal is a
photo-based intergenerational survey
method developed by Sense of Place staff
members, which can be adapted to various
communities living in different desert
habitats. It focuses on knowledge of
animals which are either unique (endemic)
to the region, or ones which are
particularly important in native
cultures subsistence, ceremonies
and beliefs. In this article, I describe
the methodology and results of a survey
conducted in the Seri communities of El
Desemboque and Punta Chueca, Sonora in
1996 and 1997.
Background
information on the seri
The Seri Indians of
Sonora, Mexico who refer to
themselves as Kunkaak (also spelled
Comcaac) are considered one
of the last surviving hunter-gatherer
groups in North America. Formerly
numbering in the thousands, this
non-agricultural people once migrated
seasonally among the islands and mainland
of the Sonoran coast, relying solely on
the naturally occurring resources of the
desert and sea for their subsistence
(Spicer 1962; Bahre 1967; Bowen 1976;
Felger and Moser 1985). Although they
have endured many socioeconomic changes
over the last century, a strong sense of
cultural identity has persisted in Seri
communities. Felger and Moser (1985),
whose ethnobotanical study of the Seri
documents their vast knowledge of Sonoran
Desert flora, noted that the Seri have
continued to rely upon traditional
knowledge for a considerable portion of
their diet and medicines. Knowledge about
the fauna has also remained critical to
their survival.
However, the accelerated
modernization of the twentieth century
may be threatening these traditional
funds of knowledge. Within
the past forty years, changes in access
to technology and in the environment have
impacted the traditional lifestyle more
drastically than two hundred years of
hostilities with Spanish conquerors and
Mexican ranchers. Today the Seri no
longer depend solely on the desert and
the sea for their survival. The majority
now lives in the permanent villages of
Punta Chueca and El Desemboque, which
were constructed by the Mexican
government in the l950s. Access to water
is no longer as problematic as it was in
the past, when depletion of the resource
necessitated moving camp. Roads link
these villages to commercial centers
where supplies and medical care can be
obtained. Food can also be purchased at
the numerous local stores, most of which
are owned by Mexicans.
Each village has a
kindergarten, an elementary school, and a
telesecundaria which delivers televised
national curriculum for students in
grades seven through nine. Most children
attend school through sixth grade,
consequently spending little time in the
desert gathering food and firewood.
Although Spanish is the official language
used in the schools, the Seri are in a
relatively strong position in terms of
language preservation. Seri (aslo known
as cmique iitom) is still the first
language in virtually all Seri
households. The sole exceptions are the
few families in which the mother is
Mexican and the father is Seri. Spanish
is used for the purpose of communicating
with outsiders, but most interactions
among the Seri are done in their
language. The children speak Seri among
themselves. Even at school, where Spanish
is the main language, several of the
classrooms are bilingual. The 1997
introduction of a textbook written in
Seri represents a major change; only
recently has a standard alphabet and
orthography been used by Seri educators.
The survey
In the words of
sociolinguist Kenneth Hale (1992:36),
language ... embodies the
intellectual wealth of the people who use
it. With this in mind, I wanted to
determine not only whether people were
familiar with the animals in the survey,
but if they knew the names of the animals
in the Seri language.
Individuals were shown
5 x 7 color photographs of
forty species of animals, selected by
Desert Museum staff for their
identifiability, their presumed cultural
importance, and their ecological
representativeness of the Sonoran Desert
fauna. Most but not all of these animals
were photographed in their natural
habitats. Community members were asked to
identify each species and to answer
specific questions about them. Interviews
were conducted in both Spanish and Seri;
each question was asked in both languages
and the ensuing discussion was either
conducted in Spanish or was translated
into Spanish for my benefit. I conducted
twenty-nine interviews, eight of which
were focus group interviews involving two
or more individuals. Of those eight group
interviews, six involved pairs of
individuals who were either sisters or
married couples. The other two group
interviews were done with different
classes at school.
Individuals were asked to
identify each species by its Seri name.
Spanish names were also recorded. In
cases in which a species was known by
several names, individuals were prompted
to give all the names they knew. In
addition to species identification, I
asked individuals which animals were
eaten, which were poisonous, which they
knew a song about, and which they liked.
I also asked questions focusing on
cultural, biological, and ecological
aspects of specific species.
For species
identification, I handed the interviewee
one photograph at a time and asked for
the name. The photographs were ordered in
such a way that morphologically similar
species (i.e. the mainland and San
Esteban Island chuckwallas, and larval
stages of two hawkmoths) were placed next
to one another. After obtaining an
identification for the first of two
similar species, I would show the
interviewee the next species and ask him
or her to compare the two. Frequently the
individual would modify the original
answer at this point.
After the identifications
were completed, I would hand the stack of
photographs to the interviewee and ask
him or her to sort through them in answer
to the various questions I asked. This
method proved more efficient than asking
each of the questions for each individual
species during the identifications. It
also seemed to engage the participants
actively and to hold their attention
longer. Finally, this technique
circumvented language difficulties,
particularly with some of the elders who
were not fluent in Spanish.
Criteria for
selecting native species for discussion
The Sonoran Desert
bioregion and, more specifically, Seri
territory within that region, is
characterized by a high degree of
endemism of flora and fauna. The forty
species in the survey are far from
comprehensive. They were selected,
rather, as representatives of these
characteristic life forms and range from
easily identifiable charismatic
mega-fauna, such as sea lions and
javelina, to the less distinctive species
such as antlions and lesser night hawks.
Nine of the species are mammals, eleven
are birds, eleven are reptiles, two are
amphibians, and seven are invertebrates.
Five of the species are marine. It was
not possible in all cases to identify
species by the photos, but the
morphological characteristics of the
organisms genus were very obvious.
Species were also chosen
on the basis of presumed cultural
salience based on work done by other
researchers (Malkin 1962; Felger and
Moser 1985) and preliminary interviews
conducted by Gary Nabhan, Howard Lawler
and others. Although the selection of
photos was meant to serve educators in
Indian, Mexican and Anglo communities, I
will focus on attributes that made them
particularly relevant in Seri
communities. Eleven of the species were
important sources of nutrition for the
Seri, and many are still consumed today.
Twenty-five species are mentioned in
traditional songs, and at least three
species are associated with specific
cultural beliefs. For example, the coral
snake was used in a traditional game
.Finally, two species of
birds, the caracara and the phainopepla
were included in the survey even though
they are not found in the immediate area.
The inclusion of extralocal unfamiliars
is useful in determining the willingness
of individuals to admit unfamiliarity
with a species. The Sonoran pronghorn is
an endangered subspecies which no longer
occurs in Seri country but was included
in the survey. Although this species was
locally extirpated around the turn of the
century and occurs only 120 kilometers
north of Seri villages today, it was
recognized by many of the elders.
Biographical data
about survey respondents
Responses from
individuals were recorded on data sheets
which also included the following
biographical information: age, years of
schooling, and degree of bilingualism,
place(s) of residence. When accurate
birthdates were not available, I
estimated them by attempting to place the
date within an historical context or in
relation to the birthdate of another
individual. The amount of schooling is
also approximate in many cases;
attendance was often disrupted for part
of the year when the families would
relocate to fishing camps. Furthermore,
at least one individual in the sample
attended school for less than one year
but later was taught to read and write by
a missionary who lived in the community.
I determined the degree of bilingualism
in casual conversation and gave each
individual a score. To determine the
potential effect of exposure to other
regions, I also noted the
individuals place of residence and
the amount of traveling that each person
had done outside the Seri community.
Between October 1996 and
April 1997, I surveyed forty-one
individuals ranging in age from 9 to 80
or more years. Representatives from both
villages were included, although the
majority lived in El Desemboque, where I
rented a house. Because several families
maintained residences in both villages, I
noted only the primary place of residence
during the interviewing period. However,
it is important to note that these are
not two distinct communities, and many
individuals have ived in both villages.
All of the interviewees
spoke Seri as their first language.
Everyone understood and spoke Spanish to
some degree and the majority was able to
communicate in it with a fair level of
competence. The years of schooling ranged
from 0 to 10. There was no positive
correlation between amount of schooling
and age or degree of bilingualism,
although only one individual over the age
of fifty had attended school.
Survey results
Compared to studies of
Oodham and Yaqui youth (Nabhan and
St. Antoine 1993), I did not find a
dramatic decrease in biosystematic
knowledge among Seri youth. They were
able to accurately identify most of the
species, and could answer questions about
easily observable phenomena. Nearly one
quarter of the species in the survey were
consensus identified by one hundred
percent of the people I interviewed.
There was a marked difference in their
ability to amply answer specific
ecological questions about habitat and
feeding preferences of the less-visible
species, however, and they tended to give
more general responses (i.e.
lizard vs. zebra-tailed
lizard) than the elders.
Furthermore, the older generation was
considerably better-versed in cultural
knowledge such as traditions, songs, and
legends about the animals.
Conclusions
The Seri survey suggests
that while other kinds of sense of
place information is not
necessarily being passed down to younger
generations in Punta Chueca and El
Desemboque, the youth do know and use the
Seri names for the local fauna, and still
talk about these animals in their native
language. Merely teaching Seri names for
animals need not be the focus of
culturally-based environmental education
in Seri villages as much as other topics
should be. For instance, songs and
certain culturally-based beliefs are not
being fully passed down. Fortunately, the
Ethnobiology and Conservation Team, with
assistance from Arizona-Sonora Desert
Museum staff and the Smithsonian
Institution, has made archival-quality
recordings of Seri songs about animals
for use in the Seri schools, and for
general distribution to Seri families, as
a means to promote appreciation and
retention of the natural history lore
embedded in Seri oral traditions. Field
trips with elders and schoolchildren to
former traditional hunting and gathering
sites have also provided a means to
transmit such sense of place lore.
Finally, the stories,
drawings and songs presented by Seri
schoolchildren themselves are being
edited into a booklet for distribution to
all Seri classrooms, as a means of
reinforcing the oral transmission of
traditional knowledge. These are but a
few of the collaborations that the Seri
community is involved with to ensure that
its rich cultural legacy of desert and
sea persists.
Literature
cited
- Bahre, C. 1967. The
Reduction of Seri Indian Range
and Residence in the State of
Sonora, Mexico, 1536 - present.
Masters Thesis. Tucson,
University of Arizona.
- Bowen, T. 1976.
Estado Actual de la Arqueología
en la Costa Central. Pages
347-364 in Sonora:
Antropología del Desierto:
Primera Reunion de Antropología
e Historia del Noroeste.
Mexico, Centro Regional del
Noroeste.
- Felger, R.S. and
M.B. Moser. 1985. People of
the Desert and Sea: Ethnobotany
of the Seri Indians. Tucson,
University of Arizona Press.
- Malkin, B. 1962. Seri
Ethnozoology. Occasional
Papers of the Idaho State College
Museum number 7.
- Nabhan, G.P. and
S. St. Antoine. 1993. The Loss of
Floral and Faunal Story: The
Extinction of Experience. Pages
229 250 in S. Kellert and
E.O. Wilson, editors, The
Biophilia Hypothesis.
Covelo, Island Press.
- Spicer, E.H. 1962.
Cycles of Conquest: The
Impact of Spain, Mexico, and the
United States on the Indians of
the Southwest, 1533 - 1960.
Tucson, University of Arizona
Press.
This
article is based on a thesis by
Janice Rosenberg, who completed her
Masters degree at the
University of Arizona in 1997. For
additional information
including a list of interview
questions used in her study
she may be contacted through Gary
Paul Nabhan at the Arizona-Sonora
Desert Museum
.BACK
|